Skip to main content

The extreme self-sacrificial behavior found in suicide bombers and soldiers presents an evolutionary puzzle: how can a trait that calls for an individual to make the ultimate sacrifice, especially in defense of a group of non-family members, persist over evolutionary time?

A new study from the National Institute for Mathematical and Biological Synthesis (NIMBioS), based at UT, provides some answers, lending insight into the causes of self-sacrifice in violent conflicts around the world from holy wars to land violence.

The study, published today in Nature’s Scientific Reports, found that sharing strong negative experiences can enable extreme cooperation in a group and, under some conditions, make individuals willing to die for the sake of the group. The researchers theorize that human evolution explains the origin of this visceral bonding over shared painful experiences, leading to an extreme type of cooperation.

The research draws on a large body of experimental evidence that suggests willingness to fight and die for a group may be motivated by what is called “identity fusion,” where personal and social identities blur, producing a sense of oneness with the group that can transcend even the bonds of kinship.

The research was conducted by a team of international researchers from UT and NIMBioS; the University of Oxford; the University of Melbourne, Australia; National University of Distance Education, Madrid; Queen’s University, Belfast; the University of London;d and RAND Corporation.

The researchers were interested in what causes identity fusion and wanted to test preliminary psychological evidence suggesting that one powerful cause of identity fusion is sharing dysphoric—painful and frightening—experiences.

They proposed that willingness to perform costly acts for the group is a behavioral strategy that evolved in human ancestors in order to succeed in high-risk collective activities and between-group conflicts. Groups whose members fused together after sharing painful experiences would be more likely to prevail in between-group conflicts, whereas ancestral groups that did not fuse would be less likely to survive. In modern groups, the willingness to sacrifice for the group would be expressed only under extreme conditions, the researchers surmised.

The study used mathematical models to generate a series of hypotheses tested in experiments.

“Using a mathematical modeling approach to explain this problem enabled us to develop new insights and knowledge that was not accessible before,” said co-author Sergey Gavrilets, a UT professor of ecology and evolutionary biology and mathematics and NIMBioS associate director for scientific activities.

There were five hypotheses: shared experience promotes willingness to perform extreme pro-group action, shared negative experiences make individuals contribute more than euphoric experiences, the more intense the experience the stronger the pro-social effects, the effect of shared negative experiences on prosocial behavior is much stronger where groups compete directly against other groups rather than cooperating against nature, and the effects of shared negative experience can be stronger than those of kinship.

The hypotheses were then tested empirically to examine the level of fusion in a variety of different study populations, including US military veterans of the Vietnam War, college fraternity and sorority members who had undergone hazing, English Premier League football fans, martial arts practitioners of Brazilian jujitsu who sometimes use painful belt whipping, and twins.

From both the theoretical and empirical research, the study concludes that overall shared negative experiences are a powerful mechanism for promoting prosocial behaviors, which under certain conditions can be extremely costly to the individuals concerned.

The models were intended to capture the conditions faced by human ancestors tens of thousands of years ago. The researchers speculate that under threatening conditions, having a shared evolutionary future likely was a more decisive factor in cooperation and self-sacrifice than shared ancestry. Adaptation and survival were particularly difficult during the Late Pleistocene more than 100,000 years ago, they noted, when the environment was highly variable and competition for limited resources was high.

“While previous research in evolutionary biology has identified a number of paths for the evolution of cooperation, this study introduces a novel, previously underappreciated but very powerful mechanism: conditioning acts of extreme cooperation on shared prior experience,” UT’s Gavrilets said.

In the study, the new mechanism was evident beyond the effects of group performance or the effects of kinship on cooperation, he said.

Gavrilets noted the common expression “band of brothers,” describing military and other units engaged in violent conflict. “Shared dysphoric experiences can make individuals feel even closer than brothers,” he said.

NIMBioS is a National Science Foundation­–supported center that brings together researchers from around the world to collaborate across disciplinary boundaries to investigate solutions to basic and applied problems in the life sciences.

 

CONTACT:

Sergey Gavrilets (865-974-8136, sergey@nimbios.org)

Catherine Crawley (865-974-9350, ccrawley@nimbios.org)